Community psychologists are very
interested in understanding cultural ways of knowing. The
concept of ‘ways of knowing’ is meant to indicate that our
outlook on life is greatly impacted by how we are raised in our
culture—the things we think about, how we think about these things,
different ways we define a problem, different solutions we might come
up with, etc. That is not to say that everyone within a culture
thinks exactly the same.
A quick exercise:
- If you are American, think for a minute and define “American culture.” Do you fit this definition?
- Now pretend you are not American, and define “American culture.” How well do you fit this definition?
[If you are not
American, substitute your own country or culture of your choice.]
As an American, I can think of a
zillion ways in which I am not your average American. But when I
place myself in the shoes of an outsider looking in, oh yeah, I can
see a zillion ways in which I’m a pretty typical American. These
are different ways of knowing. In studying cultural ways of knowing,
patterns emerge. Yes, huge variations also exist, but cultural ways
of knowing can help us to understand both the patterns and the
variations. It’s all a matter of the outlook you have, and
something psychologists like to call “the problem definition.”
One of my favorite examples of cultural
divergences in the problem definition comes from a study done in the
late 1960s. Greenfield (1997) summarized it this way:
“Cole, Gay,
Glick, and Sharp (1971) took an object-sorting task to Liberia, where
they presented it to their Kpelle participants. There were 20 objects
that divided evenly into the linguistic categories of food,
implements, food containers, and clothing. Instead of doing the
taxonomic sorts expected by the researchers, participants
persistently made functional pairings (Glick, 1968). For example,
rather than sorting objects into groups of tools and foods,
participants would put a potato and a knife together because “you
take the knife and cut the potato” (Cole et al., 1971, pg. 79).
According to Glick, participants often justified their pairings by
stating “that a wise man could only do such and such” (Glick,
1968, p. 13). In total exasperation, the researchers “finally said,
‘How would a fool do it?’ The result was a set of nice
linguistically ordered categories—four of them with five items
each” (Glick, 1968, p. 13). In short, the researchers’ criterion
for intelligent behavior was the participants’ criterion for
foolish; the participants’ criterion for wise
behavior was the researchers’ criterion for stupid.”
In this example, researchers and
participants were all clearly able to understand and demonstrate all
possible solutions to the task. But because of their different
cultural ways of knowing, they tended to define the problem and the
solution in different ways. Their initial assumptions were
different, and so they tended to draw different conclusions about
what they should do.
Another quick exercise:
- Google ‘climate change’ and read any article or watch any video clip that pops up.
- Scroll down to the comments and read them until you feel your brain turn to pudding.
You may have noticed that people think
about climate change very differently. We have different ways of
defining the problem (e.g., global threat of environmental
catastrophe, liberal conspiracy to rule the earth, real thing that’s
happening but no biggie, etc.). With each of these problem
definitions comes a separate set of solutions (e.g., large-scale
mitigation and adaptation efforts, decentralize power and let the
free market reign, do nothing because it’ll sort itself out, etc.).
There is actually a scientific
consensus on climate change that has been consistent for multiple
decades. Plenty of uncertainties remain, including how bad the
problem is going to be depending on what people decided to do. Emit
more carbon? Big problem. Lower emissions? Less big problem. But
there is so much misinformation out there, and so much more
poorly understood information that climate scientists are
really beginning to struggle with questions about communicating their
research and findings to the general public. How best to translate
science jargon into human speak? But just as important, scientists
are realizing that, as scientists, they make a series of assumptions
about knowledge, research, and data that non-scientists don’t
necessarily make. Their ways of knowing are different. Much
like the researchers trying to get the Kpelle to categorize objects
in a specific way, climate scientists would like non-scientists to
understand their data in a certain way so as to come to the same
conclusions
Because of our history of work with
culture and ways of knowing, community psychologists can contribute
to climate change communication. A few examples have recently emerged
in this aspect of the psychology of climate change. The
American Climate & Environmental Values Survey found a number
of ways of knowing that influence Americans’ understanding of and
response to climate change. For example, “We’re not ready to
abandon the American Dream” (p. 16). Communicating climate change
research within the framework of sacrifice, without also focusing on
potentials for positive development, turns Americans off. Also,
Americans often respond to one of two types of morality: ‘we should
address climate change because it’s the right thing to do,’ or,
‘we should address climate change before disasters harm our
environment, and thus, harm us.’
Another recent report, How
to Talk About Climate Change and Oceans: A FrameWorks Message Brief,
discusses specifically the ways Americans feel out of step with the
scientific community on climate change. For example, “They remain
woefully ignorant of how exactly global warming works” (p.
1, italics in original) even though they have plenty of examples of
it happening at home and abroad. Many Americans require a
foundational tutorial about how the climate operates normally in
order to understand how and why it might be changing. But take care
in communicating this because, “When scientific data is
presented in ways that seem exaggerated or overstated, Americans
become more skeptical of claims about the origins of the problem
and more likely to believe that the problem could be natural and not
anthropogenic” (p. 2, italics in original).
Climate change is a very broad field
that is so very complex. Community psychologists can positively
contribute to this field by exploring people’s ways of knowing when
it comes to the climate sciences. As mentioned before, huge
variations will always exist within a culture, even within our
cultural patters. But if we work toward an understanding of the
climate change problem definition, we can begin to agree on action
steps leading to solutions.
Greenfield, P. M. (1997). You can’t take it with you: Why ability
assessments don’t cross cultures. American Psychologist, 52(10),
1115-1124.
Citing:
Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J., & Sharp, D. W. (1971). The
cultural context of learning and thinking. New York: Basic Books.
Glick, J. (1968, February). Cognitive style among the Kpelle of
Liberia. Paper presented at the meeting on Cross-Cultural
Cognitive Studies, American Educational Research Association,
Chicago.
Kati Corlew, M.A.
University of Hawai`i, Mānoa
Thanks for a great post!
ReplyDeleteYou're more than welcomed! Thank you for reading us!
Delete